Archives of Design Research
[ Article ]
Archives of Design Research - Vol. 28, No. 4, pp.77-93
ISSN: 1226-8046 (Print) 2288-2987 (Online)
Print publication date Nov 2015
Received 16 Jul 2015 Revised 12 Aug 2015 Accepted 29 Oct 2015
DOI: https://doi.org/10.15187/adr.2015.11.28.4.77

The Effects of Eco-Feedback Design on Users’ Immediate Reactions to Water Conservation

SohnMinjung ; NamTek-Jin
Department of Industrial Design, KAIST, Daejeon, Korea Department of Industrial Design, KAIST, Daejeon, Korea

Correspondence to: Tek-Jin Nam tjnam@kaist.ac.kr

Abstract

Background Eco-feedback is a common solution for changing people’s behavior to reduce resource consumption. The design of eco-feedback becomes especially important in the context where users do not pay for the resources like in the case of public toilets. As people interact with these environments for a short time, it is critical to guide people’s behavior immediately in the moment.

Methods Our research aimed to investigate the effect of the designs of eco-feedback on users’ immediate reactions to water conservation. We conducted an in-lab experiment with 40 users on 32 designs of water consumption feedback display. To develop the design stimuli, we applied four design attributes suggested from a prior study. To present the feedback designs, a real-time water feedback system named SaveDrops was developed. While participants experienced the feedback designs, the amount of water consumption was measured by the prototype system. The participants’ reflections on the experience were gathered through surveys and post-interviews.

Results The experiment revealed that the graphical representation of water consumption was significantly more effective than the numeric representation. Frequent auditory feedback was also more effective than occasional feedback. Among the design stimuli, the most effective one for immediate water conservation was revealed as: the amount of consumed water, which was interpreted in a comparative percentage for other people, represented in a bar graph, and with beeping sound.

Conclusions This paper contributes to the sustainable design fields by investigating the effects of various eco-feedback designs on users’ immediate reactions. We expect that the designs and their effects can be good references for sustainable design practices. In particular, practitioners can refer to the design guidelines when designing products for reducing resource consumption. Also, they can select a design alternative by considering the effects.

Keywords:

Eco-feedback, sustainable interaction, design attribute, water saving, immediate reaction

1. Introduction

Sustainable interaction design (Blevis, 2007) has gained research attention in sustainable design fields, as emerging interactive products consume serious amounts of energy and resources while they are being used. Water taps, electronics, and vehicles are representative products that consume resources during product-user interaction. Sustainable interaction design techniques make users consume fewer resources with these products. Among various contexts, the facilities that many people use for a short-term period are one of the most notable application domains of sustainable interaction. These include the situations like using water in a public toilet, consuming electricity in shared spaces, and using disposable products in hotels. As users do not pay for the corresponding fees for their consumption, they tend to easily waste the resources while using the related products. In these situations, eco-feedback needs to immediately guide user behaviors, because users interact with the product only in a short-term period.

Among various strategies for sustainable interaction, we focus on the design of eco-feedback because it is known as one of the most effective and feasible strategies of sustainable interaction (Froehlich et al., 2010). We wanted to know how to improve sustainable interaction through the design of eco-feedback. Previous studies on eco-feedback verified that it is effective in reducing resource consumption (Kappel and Grechenig, 2009; Erickson et al., 2012) and raising users’ awareness about environmental impacts (Gustafsson and Gyllenswärd, 2005; Broms et al., 2010). However, as most of the studies focused on evaluating the effects of eco-feedback itself (Kappel and Grechenig, 2009) - not the effects of the “designs” of eco-feedback, the research outcomes are not applicable for using in the design process. There have been few studies to reveal the effects and the differences of the various designs of eco-feedback on people’s behavior. Prior studies compared users’ awareness or preferences to the designs through surveys and interviews. However, it is rare to evaluate behavioral changes by experiments. Thus, there is a gap between research findings of prior works and clinical knowledge for designing eco-feedback for users’ immediate reactions.

In this paper, we present an experimental study to answer how the designs of eco-feedback affect users’ immediate reaction. The aim of the study was to identify the effect of eco-feedback design on users’ immediate behavioral reactions. The eco-feedback designs were implemented according to four key design attributes of cognitive intervention (Sohn, 2015). To identify the effects of the designs of eco-feedback, we conducted an experiment focused on “water consumption.” We conducted the experiments in a laboratory to compare the effects of designs by controlling the other conditions. We developed various designs that showed the water consumption in real time and compared the effects on user behaviors and user experiences. To present the eco-feedback designs, we developed SaveDrops, a water consumption feedback display system. For the various eco-feedback designs, we developed 32 different display designs by applying four design attributes and their scales which were suggested in previous research (Sohn, 2015). As the result of experiments, we could conclude that the most effective pattern for inducing users to save water immediately was the ratio to average consumption (judgment interpretation) drawn in a bar-graph (graphical representation) with an increasing bar (neutral orientation) with frequent auditory alert (frequent degree of exposure). We also identified the differences in user experiences among the designs and discussed the implications for effective designs for water saving. Based on the understandings of user behaviors and related user experiences, we suggest the design guidelines of eco-feedback for raising the effectiveness.

This paper contributes to the sustainable design fields by understanding the effects of various eco-feedback designs on users’ immediate reaction and user experiences, according to the design attributes. We also contributed with the design of the SaveDrops system, the exemplary designs of visual and auditory eco-feedback, and the design implications. We expect that the various design patterns with their effects on users can be good references for sustainable design practices. Especially, practitioners can refer to the design method to apply the design attributes in a product, and can also select a design alternative by considering the effects.


2. Related works on Eco-Feedback

Eco-feedback is the representative field in sustainable interaction research (Froehlich et al., 2010; Strengers, 2011; Spagnolli et al., 2011). The related research in eco-feedback can be divided into three areas; i) design strategies of eco-feedback, ii) the effect of eco-feedback, and iii) the comparative effects of the different designs of eco-feedback.

2. 1. Design Strategies of Eco-Feedback

Most eco-feedback is delivered in information systems that display the information to raise users’ awareness or to change behaviors. Thus, many prior studies suggested various feedback designs by changing the content of the information, such as the factual data on the amount of consumption (Kappel and Grechenig, 2009; Kuznetsov and Paulos, 2010; Strengers, 2011), the information of historical comparisons by time (Laschke et al., 2011), and appliance-specific (Froehlich et al., 2012) or personalized information (Laschke et al., 2011). In addition to the information, some strategies have been suggested to increase the effectiveness of eco-feedback. The strategies in persuasive technology (Fogg, 2002) and motivational strategies in psychology are often discussed for applying in designing eco-feedback, such as positive and negative reinforcement (Arroyo et al., 2005), incentives and disincentives (De Young, 1993), goal setting (Abrahamse et al., 2007; McCalley et al., 2006), commitment (Abrahamse et al., 2005; De Young, 1993), and comparisons (Froehlich et al., 2010).

The strategies from other fields can raise the effectiveness when applied in the design of eco-feedback. However, it is difficult for designers to anticipate the effects of the strategies with the existing studies in this field. In addition, design practitioners could not understand all of the theories and strategies from the field of psychology. Our research is different in that the research findings can be directly applied to graphic design practice.

2. 2. Comparative Effects of Various Designs of Eco-Feedback

The empirical studies about eco-feedback evaluated mainly the impact of a single prototype. For example, Kappel and Grechenig (2009) observed through a field study that their prototype, Show-me, was effective to save about 10 liters of water in daily showers. Erickson et al. (2012) identified that it was also effective to save 6.6% of water in Dubuque city when showing the information on households’ water consumption on a portal site. In this way, eco-feedback was validated as an effective channel for actual resource saving.

However, in design fields, it could be more important to understand comparative effects of various designs for design decision-making, than just validating the effect of a single example of eco-feedback. Recently, a few studies compared several designs of eco-feedback. The representative study was UpStream (Kuznetsov and Paulos, 2010), which compared the effects of two different designs of eco-feedback on water saving. They concluded that abstract information in two colors (red and green) was more effective than numeric information, but they also found that the users did not care about the information after two days. Similarly, Kim et al. (2010) compared two different designs, named Coralog and Timelog, that show electricity consumption on a personal desktop computer in iconic and numeric designs. However, they evaluated users’ desires to save electricity instead of actual consumption changes. Froehlich et al. (2012) compared six designs of water consumption display, but they also did not evaluated users’ actual behaviors. Through surveys and interviews, they evaluated users’ preferences for each design.

In this way, most empirical studies evaluated an effect of a single design, and the comparative studies measured users’ perceptions instead of the water saving behaviors. To compare the effects of various designs, it is important to select and develop the design stimuli based on theoretical background. However, most prior studies selected the stimuli without theoretical bases. To compare the effects of the designs in detail, it is necessary to develop the design stimuli carefully after understanding the design attributes of eco-feedback and their design method.

2. 3. Design Attributes of Eco-Feedback

While design strategies indicate how to design, design attributes, indicating which characteristics of a product to design, could be more useful knowledge for designers when design a product. Several research introduced the design attributes of eco-feedback or feedback systems. Froehlich (2011) identified design spaces for eco-feedback in his doctoral dissertation. The spaces are interactivity, motivational and persuasive strategies, social aspects, comparison, data representation, display medium, information access, and actionability. Each design space includes very detailed design strategies. Fang and Hsu (2010) suggested four design dimensions by analyzing prior studies of persuasive feedback systems: ambient, aesthetic, emotionally engaged, and metaphorical. Pousman and Stasko (2006) suggested information capacity, notification level, representational fidelity, and aesthetic emphasis as design elements of ambient information. (Fang and Hsu, 2010) (Pousman and Stasko, 2006)

For the experiment of this paper, we adopted the four design attributes of cognitive intervention (Sohn, 2015) as theoretical basis for the design of eco-feedback. Sohn explained “cognitive intervention” as a type of intervention that motivated users to change their behaviors, and eco-feedback could be a type of cognitive intervention. Therefore, the design of eco-feedback inherited the design attributes of cognitive intervention. Sohn (2015) suggested four design attributes for cognitive intervention: interpretation, orientation, representation fidelity, and degree of exposure.

She explained interpretation, as “how to interpret information.” She identified that information can be interpreted in several ways, such as i) fact, ii) judgment, or iii) evaluation. Fact is the raw data that is not interpreted at all (e.g., 10 liters), judgment is interpreted information in a grade or a score (e.g., 6.3 out of 10 points), and evaluation is interpreted even more to give it a new type of value (e.g., water for saving six trees ). She explained orientation as the perspectives of information flow. The consumption amount can be expressed in a neutral way, but it also can be shown with “remained amount” that flows in a negative orientation. Representation fidelity was explained as “how realistically content is expressed.” A piece of content can be represented conceptually with texts and more realistically with photos. She described degree of exposure as how much a content is exposed to users. The frequency, duration, or size of information can be designed to control this attribute.


3. Method: In-Lab Experiment

We designed various design alternatives by applying the four design attributes of cognitive intervention, In the experimental study using the design alternatives as stimuli, we identified which pattern of eco-feedback is most effective for reducing water consumption.

3. 1. Participants

Forty university students were recruited for the experiment (19 females, average age=23.6, SD=3.61) via an online bulletin board. Each participant spent 30–50 minutes participating and was compensated with approximately US$10.

3. 2. SaveDrops: Apparatus for experiment

We developed a real-time feedback display system, SaveDrops, which shows the amount of water used in various designs. The design of this system was developed iteratively through making several prototypes.

The SaveDrops system measures the amount of water used, calculated from the flow rate. The SaveDrops system (Figure 1 and Figure 2) consists of two water flow sensors (Figure 1-c), a control kit that calculates and throws the values of sensors to a Web server (Figure 1-d), a software application that visualizes the information (Figure 1-a), and a display (Figure 1-b). Water flow sensors that measure from 1.5 to 30 liters per minute are suitable for home contexts, and two sensors are needed for cold and hot water pipes. To implement the control kit, we used an Ethernet shield with Arduino and a custom PCB. The values from the flow sensors are recorded on an SD card on the Ethernet shield and are updated on the Internet server every second. The software application was developed as a widget for Android tablet devices (10.1 inch) so that the water consumption is shown in the background. The visual designs and the sound alert interval of the feedback can be configured differently.

Figure 1

System configurations of SaveDrops

Figure 2

SaveDrops display

3. 3. Design stimulus

The feedback designs for the experiment were designed by combining different types of the identified attributes of eco-feedback. Four types of interpretation and two types of orientation, representation fidelity, and degree of exposure were designed. By combining the various types of each attribute, 32 eco-feedback designs (4 x 2 x 2 x 2) were developed. In Figure 3, 16 visual stimuli are presented out of 32 designs, which were designed by combining the scales of interpretation, representation fidelity, and orientation. All the 16 designs deliver the information that “a user consumed 8.7 liters out of 10 liters,” but they are designed in different types of expressions.

Figure 3

Sixteen design stimuli with different scales of interpretation, representation fidelity, and orientation

First, symbolic expression (Figure 3-a) and indexical expression (Figure 3-b) were used as two types of representation fidelity. For symbolic expression, the amount of water consumption was shown in numbers and texts, as they conceptually express the amount. On the other hand, indexical expression was presented in graphics that show the amount of water more realistically. It would be more realistic if it was shown in a photo or a movie, but maintained the appearance to control the stimuli in a similar design form. For two types of orientation, neutral and negative perspectives were expressed, represented as “water consumed” (Figure 3-c) and “water remaining” (Figure 3-d). The amount of “water consumed” increases from zero and the amount of “water remaining” decreases from 10 liters as water is used.

Four types of interpretation were presented as fact, judgment, environmental value, and monetary value (Figure 3). To present fact (Figure 3-e), the amount of water was displayed in liters or in 500 ml bottles. For judgment (Figure 3-f), we indicated water usage with the ratio compared to the average consumption. The number with two decimal places between 0 and 1 was displayed for symbolic expression and a horizontal bar graph for indexical expression. For the environmental value (Figure 3-g), the number of trees that can be grown with the corresponding amount of water was shown as a symbolic expression, and an oasis in the middle of desert was displayed as an indexical expression. Monetary value (Figure 3-h) was shown with the price of bottled water (about USD1 for 1 liter), instead of the real price for supplied water, to make participants more concerned about the value of water. The price was represented by towers of dimes as an indexical expression.

The degree of exposure was applied through an auditory feedback to attract the users’ attention more effectively. Two different intervals of regular auditory alerts were used for a low and high degree of exposure. Referring to the ISO standards of alarm design and studies on auditory warning (Mondor and Finley, 2003; Wogalter et al., 2002), the frequent alert was given for every 200 ml, and the occasional alert for every 2 liters of water used. The sound was a simple beeping that is classified as a basic warning sound in the sound effect inventory

3. 4. Task and Context: Washing Dishes at a Camping Site

The task of the experiment was washing dishes. We chose the task because it fits the context of the experiment where users can pay attention to the feedback display with little effort while completing the task. Eight utensils including cups and plates were provided repeated trials of the experiment. On average, users used about 10 liters of water in 5 to 10 minutes. This was enough to experience a new feedback design and was not tiresome to repeat it several times.

Each participant repeated four trials with different feedback designs. Before starting the main trials, they received an explanation of the experiment and washed the prepared utensils without any feedback. They then started to complete each trial with the feedbacks. The order of presenting feedback designs was defined by a balanced incomplete block design (Montgomery, 1984). As it was not feasible to repeat all 32 stimuli for each participant due to time and adaptation issues, the number of trials was limited to a feasible level. To ensure users experienced all scales of each attribute at least once, we decided to conduct four trials for each participant. 40 blocks were created and consisted of the design stimuli evenly in terms of each attribute. This was automatically generated by a statistics program. The orders of stimuli were also evenly randomized. For example, a participant experienced once each four types of interpretation; and twice each two types of representation fidelity, orientation, and degree of exposure (see Table 1).

Example of presenting design stimuli for four trials

Questionnaires were followed to evaluate each feedback design immediately after each trial. After finishing all four trials, an interview was followed to discuss the most preferred design and the pros and cons of each feedback design.

3. 5. Measures

Regarding participants’ behavioral reactions, their instant behavioral reactions to the prototype were observed while receiving various feedback designs. The behaviors of participants were observed in comparison with how they behaved under the condition of normal water use, with particular attention to how much they control the water flow and how often they turn off the water tap. We interviewed participants about these experiences after completing the four trials. As a result of those behavioral reactions, two variables were measured: the amount of water used and time spent due to water flow. These two measures were automatically logged by the SaveDrops system. For analyzing water consumption, the raw data (in liters) were converted to the reduction ratio (in percentage) compared to the water use amount without feedback, because the absolute values of water consumption were largely varied according to the individuals.

User experience was measured with three criteria: usability, usefulness, and satisfaction, which are basic criteria for evaluating feedback as an interactive interface. The four questions they were asked were edited from literatures (Davis, 1989; Lund, 2001). In addition to the user experience, the intrusiveness, which is the main characteristic of interventions, was also measured to verify if users also perceived the intervention with the same level of intrusiveness as the designers intended. It was also measured with four questions by editing the survey questions from existing studies (Li et al., 2002; Strengers, 2011). The 16 questions about usability, usefulness, satisfaction, and intrusiveness were evaluated with 7-point Likert scales. All the quantitative data from the water using task and questionnaires were analyzed by ANOVA. To prevent over-testing by four independent variables (Cairns, 2007), we also tested reduced models by removing irrelevant attributes and interactions for each measure.


4. Result: Immediate Behavioral Reaction to Design Patterns of Cognitive Interventions

4. 1. General Effect of Presenting Cognitive Intervention

The participants consumed 9.59 liters on average (SD=4.03) to wash the prepared utensils without any feedback on water consumption. This result is confirmed in the results of the pilot study, and it verifies that 10 liters was adequate for the initial amount of remaining water for representing negative orientation. The average amount of water consumption for the 160 trials with feedback was 6.55 liters, but it was largely dispersed from 1.90 liters to 17.42 liters. As the amount of consumption was strongly related to users’ habit, the consumption data were processed into a reduction ratio (%) that was compared to the consumption amount without feedback. On average, they reduced 29.22% (SD=23.48) from the amount in normal status without feedback.

4. 2. Effect on Water Saving and User Experiences by Design Patterns

By the levels of interpretation, analysis of variance revealed that there were no significant differences with regard to water saving. However, the differences were statistically significant (Figure 4) in usability (F=4.834, p=0.003), usefulness (F=6.024, p=0.001), and satisfaction (F=4.213, p=0.007). It means that, while the different types of interpretations were perceived differently by the participants, it did not affect the participants’ behaviors with regard to water consumption. The Tukey post-hoc test revealed that the design patterns of environmental value were evaluated lower than the other types of interpretations in usability and usefulness. This presenting fact is the most satisfactory and is ranked significantly higher than environmental value and monetary value.

Figure 4

Differences by interpretations (N=40)

Regarding the two levels of representation fidelity, graphical expression was more effective in saving water consumption (F=7.028, p=0.008) (Figure 5). Participants saved more water when receiving feedback with graphical expressions (m=31.78%, SD=22.51) than with numeric expressions (m=26.66%, SD=24.13). However, the other measures of user experiences were not statistically significant. Regarding degree of exposure, frequent auditory alerts (m=31.69%, SD=22.83) were more effective for saving water (F=6.546, p=0.010) than occasional alerts (m=26.75, SD=23.85). However, as expected, the frequent alert was significantly less satisfactory than the occasional alert (F=7.988, p=0.006). Regarding orientation, there were no statistically significant differences among all the measures. However, there were several interaction effects with other attributes; these will be explained in the next chapters.

Figure 5

Differences by representation fidelity (left) and degree of exposure (right) (N=40)

4. 3. Perceived Intrusiveness

The stimuli were designed to be less or more intervening, but users did not perceive the intrusiveness as designed. The differences among the scales were observed and they were statistically significant. As expected, the interventions designed with the higher level of scales were more intrusive in two of four attributes (Figure 6). In interpretation, presenting in monetary value was perceived as the most intrusive pattern. It was significantly more intrusive than the patterns of fact and environmental value (F=3.758, p=0.013). In addition, the frequent alerts were perceived as definitely more intrusive than occasional alerts (F=12.997, p=0.000).

Figure 6

Comparing perceived intrusiveness among the scales of each attribute (N=40)

However, higher levels of intervention were not evaluated as more intrusive in the other attributes; orientation and representation fidelity. There were no significant differences in orientation, and it resulted in an opposite way with representation fidelity. The participants answered that the numeric representation was less intrusive than graphical representation (F=8.126, p=0.005).

4. 4. The Most Effective Design Patterns on Water Saving

Considering the comparative results and the significant interaction that effects among attributes, most effective design for water saving and most usable design could be selected (Table 2). The rest could not be concluded into a specific design with all the four attributes, but we could suggest the design directions with several attributes.

The most or the least effective design patterns for each criterion

The most effective design pattern for saving water was a pattern of Judgment–Graphical–Neutral–Frequent (Figure 7-a). Analysis of variance revealed that the graphical expression and the frequent alerts were more effective than numeric expression and occasional alert for water saving (F=8.126, p=0.005 / F=6.546, p=0.012). Although there are no significant differences among the levels of interpretation and orientation, the most effective levels could be selected by referring to interaction effects. The interaction effects between interpretation and representation fidelity (F=2.730, p=0.048) revealed that judgment is more effective for graphical expression. Concerning the marginal interaction effect between orientation and degree of exposure (F=3.658, p=0.059), neutral orientation was revealed as more effective for the frequent alerts.

Figure 7

The most effective designs

From the results of questionnaires, we also extracted the most and least effective patterns for other measures. For usability, Judgment-Graphical was the most usable pattern according to the interaction effect between interpretation and representation fidelity. Likewise, an interaction effect between orientation and degree of exposure revealed that the Neutral-Occasional pattern was the most usable one (Figure 7-b).

In terms of intrusiveness, representing monetary value in numeric expressions with frequent alerts was the most intrusive pattern as stated in the results of in-lab experiment. We could not decide the specific patterns that were the most useful or satisfactory from the results. The results only revealed that presenting an environmental value was least useful and the fact that occasional alerts were the most satisfactory.

4. 5. Qualitative Results

Among the four types of interpretation, participants answered that they preferred the design patterns presented in fact and monetary value the most (30%, respectively). They explained that designs that presented fact were preferable because liters and PET bottles were the most familiar measures and metaphors. They also preferred the monetary value feedback because of its effectiveness in helping them understand the practical value of water. About the judgment feedback, there were two different opinions. The participants who preferred it answered that it was effective because they could check their consumption compared to other people and it made them want to win. They felt relieved and satisfied when they confirmed that they consumed less water than standard. However, the participants who used much less water did not prefer this design, because they thought the standard amount of water other people used was meaningless to them. The feedback of environmental value was the least preferred design. Most participants did not prefer this design mainly because they did not empathize with the contents. However, 10% of participants answered that they liked this design most because they thought the environmental value was very important, even more so than its monetary value.

As a result of observing participants’ behaviors through the trials, we found that they tended to immediately react to the sound effect in the beginning. When they heard the sound, they turned off the water, lowered the water flow, or started to wash dishes much faster. Especially with the frequent alerts, more participants reacted to the sounds, which resulted in them saving more water. Participants’ behaviors were also aligned with the results of interviews. They answered that the alert was very annoying at first, regardless of interval. However, we also observed that the impact of regular alerts decreased in a short time. In the interview, only a few participants answered that the auditory feedback was still intrusive, while most participants indicated that they were accustomed to the sounds after the four trials. Some participants also mentioned that they intentionally ignored the sounds. Thus, we can conclude that regular auditory feedback is very effective in the beginning but the effect decreases in a short time.


5. Discussion

To sum up the results of in-lab experiment, we could suggest the effects of the different designs of eco-feedback in terms of the four attributes as shown in Table 3. For example, if an eco-feedback is interpreted in monetary value, it would be more intrusive compared to other scales of interpretation. The design concepts used for experiment and the results could be practical references for designing with intervention framework.

The effects of the scales of each attribute of cognitive intervention

We discuss how our findings are differ from previous works on eco-feedback study. We also suggest eco-feedback design guidelines to be applied for effective water saving followed by the discussion of the limitation of this study.

5. 1. What are Differences from the Results of Previous User Studies?

We could find that parts of the results in this study were confirmed with existing studies. For example, the result of the experiment showed that numeric expression was more effective for saving water than graphical expression, which corresponds with the results of prior studies (Kim et al., 2010; Kuznetsov and Paulos, 2010). The result that users require exact numeric data after using graphical expression is similarly revealed in a prior study (Kim et al., 2010). However, few studies focused on the other design attributes of eco-feedback that were revealed in this study.

One of the interesting results is that the effect on water saving in this study was higher than prior studies on eco-feedback. The participants in this study saved 29.2% of water in the experiment and the interventions were effective for 90% of participants in reducing water consumption. It is higher compared to the result of prior studies such as saving 10 liters from 49–96 liters (Kappel and Grechenig, 2009), saving 2.1–2.7 gallons of 7–18 (Kuznetsov and Paulos, 2010), and changing the behavior of 49% of participants (Erickson et al., 2012). This improvement may result from three factors. The real-time feedback might cause immediate reactions, and auditory alerts could also make them focus on the feedback even when they did not watch it. Moreover, they were easily attracted to the display while washing dishes because it was installed within their line of sight.

5. 2. How to Design for Effective Water Saving

As this research focused on the water consuming context in empirical studies, the design guidelines for designing an eco-feedback for effective water saving could be determined.

5. 2. 1. Designing Effective Visual Feedback

The most effective design varied according to different user groups and contexts, although the Judgment–Graphical–Neutral–Frequent type was revealed as the most effective design from the experiment. We expect that the effect of presenting fact would be more constant with various users than other types of interventions, because its usability, usefulness, and satisfaction were higher than other types of interpretation, even though its water-saving effect was not remarkable. In other words, to express water consumption with familiar measurements or metaphors such as liters or bottles would be widely effective, regardless of individual values. This design could be applied to facilities that various users visit regularly (e.g., toilets in an office building or public showers).

In the initial stages of providing feedback, providing judgmental information that compares the user to other people would be effective for those who waste water, and presenting the environmental value would be effective for those who are trying to save water. The judgment type of information was revealed as the most effective type from the experiment, but the users who wasted water in particular tended to be more impressed with this type. These users tried to save water in order to stay below the average water use of other people, like a game, while the users who used much less water than average did not try harder with this information type. Therefore, it would be better to differentiate the settings according to the individuals’ consumption amount.

Regarding the orientation of feedback, the side effects of expressing in negative orientation needed to be carefully considered. For example, to present the remaining water for negative orientation, a certain starting point may be required. With this starting point, we could observe the same phenomenon from the experiment. The participants tended not to try hard to save water because they were relieved until it ran out. In addition, when they consumed more than the preset amount, they tended to give up trying to save water and thought that the preset amount was not enough for them.

5. 2. 2. Usefulness of Auditory Feedback and its Design

Auditory feedback was revealed as effective for saving water from the experiment, as many previous research discussed (Mondor and Finley, 2003; Wogalter et al., 2002). Participants immediately reacted to the sound alert in the beginning. However, since they adapted to the regular auditory feedback very quickly- even after four trials of several minutes, it would be better to use auditory alerts sporadically. Most participants in the experiment answered that they did not mind the sound alert. Therefore, it is important to design sound effects that cannot be easily adapted to. It would be more effective to provide auditory feedback only when users’ attention to the display is required, similar to the feedback of a refrigerator, alerting the user when the door remains open for a long time.

5. 2. 3. Provide Possibilities for Voluntary Goal-Setting

I observed that the participants set their own goal without any goal-setting function within the SaveDrops system, which helped change their consumption behaviors. Several participants set a goal while dishwashing during the experiment, even though each trial only lasted a few minutes. For example, some participants planned i) to use half or three-quarters of the preset average in the judgment pattern, ii) not to fill whole bottles of water in the Fact-Graphic-Neutral pattern, or iii) to remain at 2 liters out of 10 liters in the Fact-Numeric-Negative pattern. Although I cannot verify whether voluntary goal-setting resulted in statistical differences, participants tended to meet their goals as mentioned in the interviews.

As every user’s needs vary in diverse contexts, it is very difficult for a system to provide a certain preset goal. Instead, it would be more effective to provide basic contents that help users precisely understand the status of their consumption and behaviors so that they can set individual goals for their various situations.

5. 2. 4. Designing a Customized Information Display

According to the result of the in-lab experiments, it would be more effective to provide judgmental information for those who waste water and to present the environmental value for those who are trying to save water. The judgment pattern, compared to other people, was the most effective pattern in total, but it was revealed that the users who wasted water were more impressed with this pattern. They tried to save water as not to exceed the average of other people like a game, while the users who spent much less water than average did not try harder. Therefore, the settings need to be differentiated according to the individuals’ consumption amount. The design with environmental value was the least satisfactory, but several participants preferred it as the most effective pattern in the interview of the experiment. As they cared about the value of water and the biospheric impact, they were especially impressed with the designs. It would be enough to present environmental value for a week or two, or intermittently.

5. 3. Limitations

There are several limitations in our study. First, the settings of the experiment cover a limited context, For example, different types of user groups, in terms of age, gender, and culture should be evaluated in depth. Second, as the studies were aimed at immediate user reaction for water saving, the results and findings might not be suitable for other contexts that are associated with long-term behavior change. Third, our work is focused on eco-feedback design for water saving. Although designers have the ability to infer the effect of the same design pattern in other contexts, such as saving electricity, the proposed design and guidelines should be further evaluated to be practically implemented.


6. Conclusion

This research investigated the effect of various designs of eco-feedback on users’ immediate reaction in order to gain insights on the guidelines for the design of eco-feedback. We developed and compared 32 designs of water consumption feedback display by applying the four design attributes of cognitive intervention suggested from a prior study. Through in-lab experiments, we found that users consume significantly less water with graphical representation than textual representation. Moreover, frequent auditory alert was significantly more effective for saving water than occasional alert. Considering the interaction effects among the design attributes, we infer the most effective eco-feedback pattern as the information of the ratio to average consumption (judgment interpretation) drawn in a bar-graph (graphical representation) with an increasing bar (neutral orientation) with frequent auditory alert (frequent degree of exposure). Based on the results, we discussed the differences of user experiences according to the eco-feedback designs and the design implications for designing effective visual and auditory feedback.

The various design patterns of eco-feedback and their effects on users are expected to be used in design practice as a design reference and a rationale for design decision-making. The 32 exemplary designs that we suggested as the design stimuli can be used as references for practitioners when applying design attributes into the design of real products. Although this research evaluated the effects for water saving, the design patterns can be applied for the design of other products to save other types of resources, such as electricity and disposable goods. Future work includes an experimental study to identify the effects when the products that require immediate behavioral reactions are repeatedly used. The effects of the designs with various types of users, resources, and contexts also need to be investigated further.

Acknowledgments

This work was done by 2015 KAIST Research Fund.

Notes

Citation: Shon, M., & Nam, T. J. (2015).The Effects of Eco-Feedback Design on Users’ Immediate Reactions to Water Conservation. Archives of Design Research, 28(4), 77-93.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/), which permits unrestricted educational and non-commercial use, provided the original work is properly cited.

References

  • Abrahamse, W., Steg, L., Vlek, C., & Rothengatter, T. (2005). A review of intervention studies aimed at household energy conservation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25(3), 273-291. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.08.002]
  • Abrahamse, W., Steg, L., Vlek, C., & Rothengatter, T. (2007). The effect of tailored information, goal setting, and tailored feedback on household energy use, energy-related behaviors, and behavioral antecedents. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27(4), 265-276. [https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.08.002]
  • Arroyo, E., Bonanni, L., & Selker, T. (2005). Waterbot: exploring feedback and persuasive techniques at the sink. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, ACM Press, 631-639. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1054972.1055059]
  • Blevis, E. (2007). Sustainable interaction design: invention & disposal, renewal & reuse. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, ACM Press, 503-512. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1240624.1240705]
  • Broms, L., Katzeff, C., Bång, M., Nyblom., Hjelm, S. I., & Ehrnberger, K. (2010). Coffee maker patterns and the design of energy feedback artefacts. Proceedings of the 8th ACM Conference on Designing Interactive Systems, ACM Press, 93-102. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1858171.1858191]
  • Cairns, P. (2007). HCI... not as it should be: inferential statistics in HCI research. Proceedings of the 21st British HCI Group Annual Conference on People and Computers: HCI... but not as we know it-Volume 1, British Computer Society, 195-201.
  • Davis, F. D. (1989). Perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user acceptance of information technology. MIS quarterly, 319-340. [https://doi.org/10.2307/249008]
  • De Young, R. (1993). Changing behavior and making it stick The conceptualization and management of conservation behavior. Environment and Behavior, 25(3), 485-505. [https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916593253003]
  • Erickson, T., Podlaseck, M., Sahu, S., Dai, J. D., Chao, T., & Naphade, M. (2012). The dubuque water portal: evaluation of the uptake, use and impact of residential water consumption feedback. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, ACM Press, 675-684. [https://doi.org/10.1145/2207676.2207772]
  • Fang, W., & Hsu, J. (2010). Design Concerns of Persuasive Feedback System. Visual Representations and Reasoning, 20-25.
  • Fogg, B. J. (2002). Persuasive technology: using computers to change what we think and do. CA: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers. [https://doi.org/10.1145/764008.763957]
  • Froehlich, J., Findlater, L., & Landay, J. (2010). The design of eco-feedback technology. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, ACM Press, 1999-2008. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1753326.1753629]
  • Froehlich, J., Findlater, L., Ostergren, M., Ramanathan, S., Peterson, J., Wragg, I., ..., & Landay, J. A. (2012, May). The design and evaluation of prototype eco-feedback displays for fixture-level water usage data. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 2367-2376). ACM. [https://doi.org/10.1145/2207676.2208397]
  • Froehlich, J. E. (2011). Sensing and feedback of everyday activities to promote environmental behaviors. University of Washington.
  • Gustafsson, A., & Gyllenswärd, M. (2005, April). The power-aware cord: energy awareness through ambient information display. In CHI'05 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems (pp. 1423-1426). ACM. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1056808.1056932]
  • Kappel, K., & Grechenig, T. (2009, April). Show-me: water consumption at a glance to promote water conservation in the shower. In Proceedings of the 4th international conference on persuasive technology (p. 26). ACM. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1541948.1541984]
  • Kim, T., Hong, H., & Magerko, B. (2010). Designing for persuasion: toward ambient eco-visualization for awareness. In Persuasive technology (pp. 106-116). Springer Berlin Heidelberg. [https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-13226-1_12]
  • Koening, M. (2009, September). Music Censor Sound. SoundBible. Retrieved from http://soundbible.com/1013-Music-Censor.html.
  • Kuznetsov, S., & Paulos, E. (2010, April). UpStream: motivating water conservation with low-cost water flow sensing and persuasive displays. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 1851-1860). ACM. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1753326.1753604]
  • Laschke, M., Hassenzahl, M., Diefenbach, S., & Tippkämper, M. (2011, May). With a little help from a friend: a shower calendar to save water. In CHI'11 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 633-646). ACM. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1979742.1979659]
  • Li, H., Edwards, S. M., & Lee, J. H. (2002). Measuring the intrusiveness of advertisements: Scale development and validation. Journal of advertising, 31(2), 37-47. [https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2002.10673665]
  • Lund, A. M. (2001). Measuring usability with the USE questionnaire. Usability interface, 8(2), 3-6.
  • Mccalley, T., Kaiser, F., Midden, C., Keser, M., & Teunissen, M. (2006). Persuasive appliances: Goal priming and behavioral response to product-integrated energy feedback. Persuasive Technology, 45-49. [https://doi.org/10.1007/11755494_7]
  • Mondor, T. A., & Finley, G. A. (2003). The perceived urgency of auditory warning alarms used in the hospital operating room is inappropriate. Canadian journal of anaesthesia, 50(3), 221-228. [https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03017788]
  • Montgomery, D. C., & Montgomery, D. C. (1984). Design and analysis of experiments. New York: Wiley.
  • Pousman, Z., & Stasko, J. (2006). A taxonomy of ambient information systems: four patterns of design. Proceedings of the working conference on Advanced visual interfaces, ACM Press, 67-74. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1133265.1133277]
  • Sohn, M.(2015). Design Method for Sustainable Interaction: Understanding and Applying Design Attributes of Product Intervention for Immediate Behavior Change (Doctoral dissertation). KAIST, Daejeon, Republic of Korea.
  • Spagnolli, A., Corradi, N., Gamberini, L., Hoggan, E., Jacucci, G., Katzeff, C., Broms, L., & Jönsson, L. (2011). Eco-feedback on the go: Motivating energy awareness. Computer, 44(5), 38-45. [https://doi.org/10.1109/MC.2011.125]
  • Strengers, Y. A. (2011, May). Designing eco-feedback systems for everyday life. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 2135-2144). ACM. [https://doi.org/10.1145/1978942.1979252]
  • Wogalter, M. S., Conzola, V. C., & Smith-Jackson, T. L. (2002). Research-based guidelines for warning design and evaluation. Applied ergonomics, 33(3), 219-230. [https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-6870(02)00009-1]

Figure 1

Figure 1
System configurations of SaveDrops

Figure 2

Figure 2
SaveDrops display

Figure 3

Figure 3
Sixteen design stimuli with different scales of interpretation, representation fidelity, and orientation

Figure 4

Figure 4
Differences by interpretations (N=40)

Figure 5

Figure 5
Differences by representation fidelity (left) and degree of exposure (right) (N=40)

Figure 6

Figure 6
Comparing perceived intrusiveness among the scales of each attribute (N=40)

Figure 7

Figure 7
The most effective designs

Table 1

Example of presenting design stimuli for four trials

Trials Interpretation Representation fidelity Orientation Degree of exposure
1 fact indexical negative more
2 judgment symbolic negative less
3 evaluation
(environmental value)
indexical neutral less
4 evaluation
(monetary value)
symbolic neutral more

Table 2

The most or the least effective design patterns for each criterion

Criteria Interpretation Representation
fidelity
Orientation Degree of exposure
Most effective
for water saving
judgment graphical neutral frequent
Most usable judgment graphical neutral occasional
Most satisfied fact - - occasional
Least useful evaluation
(environmental value)
- - -
Most intrusive evaluation
(monetary value)
numeric - frequent

Table 3

The effects of the scales of each attribute of cognitive intervention

Attributes Scales (Design) Effects
Interpretation fact (amount) most satisfactory
least intrusive
judgment (compared to others) more effective for water saving
evaluation (environmental value) least usable
least useful
least satisfactory
evaluation (monetary value) most intrusive
Representation fidelity symbolic (textual) more intrusive
indexical (graphical) more effective for water saving
negative (remained) neutral (consumed) -
negative (remained) side effects
Degree of exposure less exposed (occasional alert) more satisfactory
more exposed (frequent alert) more effective for water saving
more intrusive